А-П

П-Я

А  Б  В  Г  Д  Е  Ж  З  И  Й  К  Л  М  Н  О  П  Р  С  Т  У  Ф  Х  Ц  Ч  Ш  Щ  Э  Ю  Я  A-Z

 

Таким обра-
зом, обычное <одномерное> предсказание исхода лече-
ния, столь часто применяемое в психотерапии, не
только не учитывает сложности человеческой индиви-
дуальности, но и умаляет способность врача постичь
эту сложность и опираться на нее в ходе терапии.
Использование процедуры предсказания индивиду-
альных решеток пациентов и сравнение этого предска-
зания с реальными результатами позволили выявить
источники конкретных ошибок психотерапевтов даже в
тех случаях, когда в целом предсказания оказывались
правильными, что даст возможность в будущем эмпири-
чески выявлять <горячие точки> терапии, до сих пор
поддававшиеся лишь ad hoc интерпретации психоанали-
тиков.
Матрицы <идеального результата психотерапии>
Эти матрицы позволили выявить пациентов, оправ-
давших ожидания психотерапевтов и не оправдавших
таких ожиданий, что уже само по себе достаточно
интересно. В пяти случаях из восьми решетки, запол-
ненные пациентами в ходе повторного тестирования,
коррелировали с решетками <идеального результата
психотерапии>. Анализ же тех матриц <идеального
результата психотерапии>, в которых нашли свое отра-
жение нереалистичные ожидания психотерапевтов, дал
возможность увидеть то, каким образом <идеализирует-
ся идеальное>. Может быть, психотерапию наряду с
политикой следует считать искусством учета всех воз-
можностей. В некоторых случаях несоответствие меж-
ду решеткой <идеального результата> и решеткой,
заполненной пациентом, свидетельствовало не о неуда-
че лечения, а о том, что пациентом был найден
альтернативный способ решения проблемы, не учтен-
ный психотерапевтом, но впоследствии как самим паци-
ентом, так и психотерапевтом признанный удовлетвори-
тельным (вспомним пациента X.).
Поэтому в тех случаях, когда был достигнут идеаль-
ный, по мнению психотерапевтов, результат, его следу-
ет интерпретировать не в терминах <правильности> или
<абсолютности предсказаний>, но в более релятивист-
ских терминах, как показатель совпадения ожиданий
психотерапевтов с реальным результатом.
Образно говоря, в данном случае мы имеем тот
<материал, из которого сделаны предсказания>,-
интуитивные догадки, которые проверяются будущими
событиями и заставляют по-новому оценить наши зна-
ния на данный момент. Диапазон качества таких дога-
док чрезвычайно широк-от смутных и неточных
высказываний, сделанных, что называется, вслепую, до
удивительно точных, обоснованных предсказаний. Не-
предсказуемым является лишь то, насколько точным
будет данное конкретное предсказание. Отсюда, кста-
ти, следует, что предсказание, сделанное с целью
лучшего контроля поведения, имеет меньше шансов на
успех по сравнению с предсказанием, цель которого-
лучше понять другого человека.
Выводы для дальнейших исследований
Во-первых, при разборе недостатков использованной
методики в дальнейших исследованиях при повторном
тестировании следует не только использовать ранее
выявленные кокструкты, но и снова повторять процеду-
ры выявления, чтобы включить в исследование вновь
возникшие конструкты. Это даст дополнительные све-
дения для сравнения результатов двух тестирований и
не нозполяет упустить из виду то качественно новое,
что появилось в феноменальном мире пациентов в
результате терапии.
Во-вторых, <Я-концепцию> лучше исследовать, ис-
пользуя представление о себе в качестве элемента, а не
конструкта.
В-третьих, как отмечалось при критике теории
концептуальной структуры, предложенной Маклуф-
Норрис и ее коллегами (148), для того чтобы оконча-
тельно изгнать из психологии представление о том, что
решетки выявляют стабильные личностные черты и что
с их помощью можно классифицировать испытуемых
по диагностическим категориям, необходимо провести
исследование, в котором решетки (наряду с беседой)
заполнялись бы пациентами периодически через каж-
дые 2-3 месяца. Весьма заманчиво также изучить
диапазон и виды изменений, происходящих с системой
конструктов с течением времени. Это дало бы возмож-
ность разработать более чувствительные индексы изме-
нений, чем те, которые используются при проведении
единственного повторного тестирования через год после
первого.
В заключение хочу сказать следующее. Я думала,
что мне удалось сделать данное исследование подлинно
рефлексивным, так как в число обследуемых наряду с
пациентами были включены и психотерапевты. Потом я
поняла, что забыла включить в это число себя. Неуже-
ли подлинная рефлексивность-недостижимый мираж?
Библиография
1. Adams-Webber, J. (1969). Cognitive complexity nnd sociality.
British Journal of social and clinical Psycilology 8, 211-216.
2. Aclams-Webbcr, .Т. (1970a). An aniilysis of the iliscriniinant validity
of several repertory grid indices. British Journal of Psychology 61, 83-90.
3. Adams-Wcbber, J. (1970b). Elicited versus provided constructs in
repertory grid technique: a review. British Journal of medical Psychology
43, 349-354.
4. Adams-Webber, J., Schwenkci, B. and Barbeau, D. (1972). Perso-
nal constructs and the perception of individual differences. Canadian
Journal of Behavioural Science, 4, 218-224.
5. Agnew, J. and Bannister, D. (1973). Psychiatric diagnosis as a
pseudo specialist language. British Journal of medical Psychology 46, 69.
6. Allisop, В, (1972). The development of personal construct systems.
Unpublished manuscript, Memorial University, St. Johns, Newfoundland,
Canada.
7. Applebee, A.N. (1975). Developmental changes in consensus in
construing within a specified domain. Britisil Journal of Psychology 66, 4
473-480.
8. Applebee, A. N. (1976). The development of childrens responses
to repertory grids. British Journal of social and clinical Psychology 15,
101-102.
9. Arthur, A.Z. (1966). Response bias in the semantic differential.
British Journal of social and clinical Psychology, 5, 103-107.
10. Baillie-Grohman, R. (1975). The use of a modified form of
repertory grid technique to investigate the extent to which deaf school
leavers tend to use stereotypes. Unpublished M.Sc. dissertation, Universi-
ty of London.
II, Bannister, D. (1959). An application of personal construct theory
(Kelly) to schi/.oid thinking, Unpublished PhD. thesis. University of
London.
12. Bannister, D. (1960). Conceptual structure in thought disordered
schizophrenics. Journal of Menial Science 106, 1230-1249.
13. Bannister, U. (1962a). The nature and measurement of schizo-
plirenic thought disorder. Journal of Mental Science 108, 825-842.
14. Bannister, 13. (1962). Personal construct theory: a summary and
experimental paradigm. Ada Psychologica 20, 104-120.
15. Bannister. D. (1963). The genesis of schizophrcnic thought
disorder: a serial invalidation hypothesis. British Journal of Psychiatry,
109, 680-686.
16. Bannister, D. (1965a). The rationale and clinical relevance of
repertory grid technique. British Journal of Psychiatry III, 977-982.
17. Bannister, D. (1965b). The genesis of schizophrcnic thought
disorder: re-test of the serial invalidation hypothesis. British Journal of
Psychiatry III, 377-382.
18. Bannister, D. and Bott, M. (1974). Evaluating the person. In
"New Approaches to Psychological Medicine" (Ed. P. Kelin), \Viley,
London.
19. Bannister, D. and Fransella, F. (1965). A repertory grid test of
schizophrenic thought disorder. British Journal of social and clinical
Psychology 2, 95-152.
20. Bannister, D. and Fransella, F. (1966). "Grid Test of Thought
DisoriJcr". Psychological Test Publications. Barnstaple.
21. Bunnister, D. and Mair, J.M.M. (1968). In "The Evaluation of
Personal Constructs". Academic Press, London.
22. Bannister, D. and Salmon, P. (1966a). Schizophrenic thought
disorder: specific or diffuse? British Journal of medical Psychology 39,
215-219.
23. Bannister, D. and Salmon, P. (]966b). A measure of intransitivi-
ty within resistance-to-change grids. Unpublished manuscript.
24. Bannister, D. and Salmon, P. (1967). Measures of superordinacy.
Unpublished manuscript.
25. Bannister, D., Fransella, F. and Agnew, J. (1971). Characteristics
and validity of the grid test of thought disorder. British Journal of social
and clinical Psychology 10, 144-151.
26. Bannister, D., Adams-Webber, J.R., Penn, W.I. and Rad-
ley, A.R. (1975). Reversing the process of thought disorder: a serial
validation experiment. British Journal of social and clinical Psychology
14, 169-180.
27. Barton, E.S., Walton, T. and Rowe, D. (1976). Using grid
technique with the mentally handicapped. In "Explorations of Interperso-
nal Space" (Ed. P. Slater), Vol. 1. Wiley, London.
28. Bateson, G.. Jackson, D., Haley, J. and Weakland, J. (1956).
Towards a theory of schizophrenia. Behavioural Science 4, 251.
29. Bender, M.P. (1969). To smile at or avert the eyes from: the
formation of relationships among students. Research in Education 2,
32-51.
30. Bender, M.P. (1974). Provided versus elicited constructs: an
explanation of Warr and Coffmans (1970) anomolous finding. British
Journal of social and clinical Psychology 13, 329.
31. Bender, M.P. (1976). Does construing people as similar involve
similar behaviour towards them? A subjective and objective replication.
British Journal of social and clinical Psychology IS, 93-95.
32. Benjafield, J. and Adams-Webber, J.R. (1975). Assirnilative
projection and construct balance in the repertory grid. British Journal of
Psychology 66, 169-173.
33. Bergin, A.E. (1972). The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes. In
"Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behaviour Change" (Eds. A.E. Bergin
and S.L. Garfield). Academic Press, London, and New York.
34. Bieri, J. (1955). Cognitive complexity-simplicity and predictive
behaviour. Journal of abnormal and social Psychology Si, 263-268.
35. Bieri, J.. Atkins, A.L., Briar, S., Leaman, R.L., Miller, H. and
Tripodi, T. (1966). "Clinical and Social Judgement: the Discrimination of
Behavioural Information". Wiley, New York.
36. Bonarius, J.C.J. (1965). Research in the personal construct theory
of George A. Kelly: role construct repertory test and basic theory. In
"Progress in Experimental Personality Research" (Ed. B.A. Maher),
Vol. 2,2-46, Academic Press, New York and London.
37. Bonarius. J.C.J. (1971). "Personal Construct Psychology and
Extreme Response Style". Swets and Zeitlinger, N. V., Amsterdam.
38. Briciley, D.\V. (1967). The use of personality constructs by
children of three different ages. Unpublished PhD. thesis, University of
London.
39. Brown, R.W. (1958). Is a boulder sweet or sour? Contemporary
Psychology 3, 113-115.
40. Caine, T.M. and Smail, D.J. (1967). Personal relevance and
choice of constructs for the repertory grid technique. British Journal of
Psychiatry 113, 517-520.
41. Caine, T.M. and Smail, D.J. (1969). A study of the reliability and
validity of the repertory grid technique as a measure of the hysteroid/ob-
sessoid component of personality. Britisll Journal of Psychiatry 115,
1305-1308.
42. Caplan, H.L., Rhode, P.O., Shapiro, D.A. and Watson, J.P.
(1975). Some correlates of repertory grid measures used to study a
psychotherapeutic group. British Journal of medical Psychology 48,
217-226.
43. Carlson, R. (1971). Sex differences in ego functioning: explorato-
ry studies of agency and communion. Journal of consulting and clinical
Psychology 37, 267-277.
44. Carr,J.E. (1970). Differentiation similarity of patient and therapist
and the outcome of psychotherapy. Journal of abnormal Psychology 76,
361-369.
45. Cattell, R.B. (1966). "Handbook of Multivariete Experimental
Psychology". Rand MeNally, Chicago.
46. Coleman, P. G. (1975). Interest in personal activities and degree
of perceived implications between personal constructs. British Journal of
social and clinical Psychology 14, 93-95.
47. Coombs, С. H. (1964). "A Theory of Data", Wiley, New York.
48. Crisp, A. H. (1964). An attempt to measure an aspect of
"Transference". British Journal of medical Psychology 37, 17-30.
49. Crockett, W. H. (1965). "Cognitive Complexity and Impression
Formation in Progress in Experimental Personality Research" (Ed.
B. A. Maher) Vol. 2. Academic Press, New York and London.
50. Cromwell, R. L. and Caldwell, D. F. (1962). A comparison of
ratings based on persona] constructs of self and others. Journal of clinical
Psycilology 18, 43-46.
51. Cronbach, L. J. (1946). Response sets and test validity.
Educational and Psychological Measurement 6, 475-494.
52. Draffan, J.W. (1973). Randomness in grid test scores. Britisll
Journal of medical Psychology 46, 391.
53. Duck, S. W. (1973). Similarity and perceived similarity of
personal constructs as influences on friendship choice. Britisll Journal of
social and clinical Psychology 12, 1-6.
54. Duck, S. and Spencer, C. (1972). Personal constructs and
friendship formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 28,
40-45.
55. Epting, F. R. (1972). The stability of cognitive complexity in
construing social issues. British Journal of social and clinical Psychology
II, 122-125.
56. Epting, F. R. (1975). Order of presentation of construct poles.
What are the factors to be considered? A reply. British Journal of social
and clinical Psychology 14, 427-428.
57. Epting, F. R., Suchman, D. 1. and Nickeson, K. J. (1971). An
evaluation of elicitation procedures for personal constructs. British
Journal of Psychology 62, 513-517.
58. Fager, R. E. (1962). Program for the analysis of repertory grids
on the 1620 IBM computer. Unpublished manuscript, Syracuse Universi-
У-
59. Feidman, M. M. (1975). The body image and object relations:
exploration of a method utilizing repertory grid techniques. Britisll Journal
of Medical Psychology 48, 317-332. ,
60. Fielding, J. M. (1975). A technique for measuring outcome in
group psychotherapy, British Journal of medical Psychology 48, 189-
198.
61. Field, S. P. and Landfield, A. W. (1961). Personal construct
theory consistency. Psychological Reports 8, 127-129.
62. Foulds, G. A., Hope, K., MePherson, F. M. and Mayo, P. R.
(1967). Cognitive disorder among the schizophrenias. In: The validity of
some tests of thought process disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 113,
1361-1368.
63. Fransella, F. (1965). The effects of imposed rhythm and certain
aspects of personality on the speech of stu.terers. Unpublished PhD.
thesis, University of London.
64. Fransella. F. (1969). The stutterer as subject or object? In
"Stuttering and the Conditioning Theraphies". (Eds. B. B. Gray and
G. England). Monterey Institute for Speech and Hearing, California.
65. Fransella, F. (1970). ... and then there was one. In "Perspectives
in Personal Construct Theory" (Ed. D. Bannistcr). Academic Press,
London and New York.
66. Fransella, F. (1972). "Personal Change and Reconstruction:
Research on a Treatment of Stuttering". Academic Press, London and
New York.
67. Fransella, F. (1974). Thinking in the obsessional. In "Obsessional
States" (Ed. H. R. Beech). Mathuen, London.
68. Fransella, F. (1975). Studying the individual. In "Methods of
Psychiatric Research" (Eds. P. Sainsbury and N. Kreitman) (2nd Ed.).
Oxford University Press, London.
69. Fransella, F. (1976). The theory and measurement of personal
constructs. In "Recent Advances in Clinical Psychiatry" (Ed. K. Granvil-
le-Grossman). Churchill Livingstone, London.
70. Fransella, F. (1977). The self and the stereotype. In "New
Perspectives in Personal Construct Theory" (Ed. D. Bannister). Academic
Press, London and New York.
71. Fransella, F. and Adams, B. (1966). An illustration of the use of
repertory grid technique in a clinical setting. British Journal of social and
clinical Psychology 5, 51-62.
72. Fransella, F. and Bannister, D. (1967). A validation of repertory
grid technique as a measure of political construing. Acta Psychologica 26,
97-106.
73. Fransella, F. and Crisp, A. H. (1970). Conceptual organisation
and weight change. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 18, 176-185.
74. Fransella, F. and Crisp, A. H. (1971). Conceptual organisation
and weight change. In "Recent Research in Psychosomatics" (Ed.
R. A. Pierlot). S. Karger, Basel.
75. Fransella, F. And Crisp, A. H. (1977). Comparisons of weight
concepts in groups of a) neurotic b) normal and c) anorexic females.
British Journal of Psychiatry (in press).
76. Fransella, F. and Joyston-Beenal, M. B. (1971). An investigation
of conceptual process and pattern change in a psychotherapy group.
British Journal of Psychiatry 119, 199-206.
77. Frith, С. E. and Lillie, F. J. (1972). Why does the repertory grid
test indicate thought disorder? British Journal of social and clinical
Psychology II, 73-78.
78. Gibson, M. (1975). An illustration of the effect of the order of
presentation of construct poles on Bieris measure of cognitive complexity.
British Journal of social and clinical Psychology 14, 425-426.
79. Guertin, W. H. (1973). SORTO: Factor analyzing Q sorts of
Kellys personal construct productions. Journal of Personality Assessment
37, 69-77.
80. Hall, M. F. (1966). The generality of cognitive complexity-
simplicity. Unpublisned doctorni dissertation, Vanderbilt University.
81. Hamilton, D. L. (1968). Personality attributes associated with
extreme response style. Psychological Bulletin 69, 192-203.
82. Harris, G. С. (1962). Some Rao-Guttman relationships. Psycho-
metrika 21, 185-190.
83. Haynes, E. Т. and Phillips, J.P.N. (1973). Inconsistency, loose
construing and schizophrenic thought disorder. British Journal of Psychi-
atry 123, 209-217.
84. Hays, W. L. (1958). An approach to the study of trait
implications and trait similarity. In "Person Perception and Interpersonal
Behaviour" (Eds. R. Taguiri and L. Petrullo). Stanford University Press,
Stanford.
85. Heather, N. (1976). The specificily of schizophrenic thought
disorder: a replication and extension of previous findings. British Journal
of social and clinical Psychology 15, 131-137.
86. Heider, F. (1946). Attitude and cognitive organisation. Journal of
Psychology 2, 107-112.
87. Hernsley, D. R. (1976). Problems in the interpretation of
cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psycluafry
129, 32-35.
88. Hinkle, D. (1965). "The Change of Personal-Consti-ucts from the
View point of a Theory of Construct Implications". Unpublished Ph. D.
thesis, Ohio State University.
89. Hjelle, L. A. and Ziegler, D. J. (1976). "Personality Theories:
Basic Assumptions, Research, and Applications". MeGraw-Hill New
York.
90. Holt, R. R. (1958). Clinical and statistical prediction: a reformula-
tion and some new data. Journal of abnormal and social Psychology 56
1-12.
91. Honess, Т. (1976). Cognitive complexity and social prediction.
British Journal of social and clinical Psychology 15, 23-31.
92. Honess, Т. (1977a). An implication grid suitable for use in
developmental research. Unpublished manuscript. University of Exeter:
93. Honess, T. (1977b). A comparison of the implication and
repertory grid techniques. Unpublished manuscript. University of Exeter.
94. Honikman, B. (1976). Construct theory as an approach to
architectural and environmental design. In "Explorations of Interpersonal
Space" (Ed. P. Slater), Vol. 1. Wiley, London.
95. Hoy, R. M. (1973). The meaning of alcoholism for alcoholics: a
repertory grid study. Britisil Journal of social and clinical Psycilology 12,
98-99.
96. Hudson, R. (1974). Images of the retailing environment: an
example of the use of the repertory grid methodology. Environmental
Behaviour 6, 470-494.
97. Hunt, D. E. (1951). Studies in role concept repertory: conceptual
consistency. Unpublished M. A. thesis, Ohio State University.
98. Isaacson G. 1. (1966) A comparative study of the meaningfulness
of personal and common constructs. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Missouri.
99. Isaacson G. 1. and Landfield A. W. (1965). Meaningfulness of
personal and common constructs. Journal of Individual Ps\cliology, 21,
160-166.
100. Judkins M. (1976). Introspective dialogue technique. Unpublished
manuscript. Royal Free Hospital, London.
101. Kear-Colwell J. J. (1973). Bannister-Fransella grid performance:
relationships with personality and intelligence. Britisil Journal of social
clinical Psychology, 12, 78-82.
102. Kelly G. A. (1955). "The Psychology of Personal Constructs",
Vols I and 2. Norton, New York.
103. Kelly, G. A. (1961). The abstraction of human processes.
Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Applied Psychology,
Munksgaard, Copenhagen.
104. Kelly G. A. (1963). Comments on Aldous, the personable
computer. In "Computer Simulation of Personality" (Eds S. Tornkins and
S. Messick). Wiley, New York.
105. Kelly G. A. (1969). The role of classification in personality
theory. In "Clinical Psychology and Personality: the Selected Papers of
George Kelly" (Ed. B. Maher). Wiley, New York.
106. Kelly G. A. (1970). Behaviour is an experiment. In "Perspecti-
ves in Personal Construct Theory" (Ed. D. Bannister). Academic Press,
London and New York.
107. Kelly J. V. (1963). A programme for processing George Kellys
Rep Grids on the IBM 1620 computer. Inpublished manuscript, Ohio State
University.
108. Kelsall. P. N. and Strongman, K. T. (1977). Emotional
experience and the implication grid. British Journal of Medical Psycholo-
gy (in press).
109. Kuusinen, J. and Nystedt, L. (1975a). The convergent validity
of four indices of cognitive complexity in person perception. Scandinavi-
an Journal of Psychology 16, 131-136.
110. Kuusinen, J. and Nystedt, L. (1975b). Individual versus
provided constructs, cognitive complexity and extremity of ratings in
person perception. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 16, 137-148.
III. Laing, R. D. and Esterson, A. (1964). "Sanity, Madness and the
Family". Tavistock, London.
112. Landfield, A. W. (1965). Meaningfulness of self, ideal and other
as related to own versus therapists personal construct dimensions.
Psychological Reports 16, 605-608.
113. Landfield, A. W. (1968). The extremity rating revisited within
the context of personal construct theory. British Journal of social and
clinical Psychology 1, 135-139.
114. Landfield, A. W. (1971). "Personal Construct Systems in
Psychotherapy". Rand MeNally, Chicago.
115. Landfield, A. W. and Barr, M. A. (1976). Ordination: a new
measure of concept organization. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Nebraska-Lincoln, U.S.A.
116. Lansdown, R. (1975). A reliability study of the 8х8 repertory
grid. Journal of the Association of Educational Psychologists.
117. Lauterbach, W. (1972). The measurement of conflict and its
correlation with mood. Unpublished Ph. D. thesis. University of London.
118. Lauterbach, W. (1975a). Assessing psychological conflict. Bri-
tish Journal of social and clinical Psychology 14, 43-47.
119. Lauterbach, W. (1975b). Covariation of conflict and mood in
depression. British Journal of social and clinical Psychology 14, 49-53.
120. Leitner, L. M., Landfield, A. W. and Barr, M. A. (1975).
Cognitive complexity: a review and elaboration within personal construct
theory. Unpublished manuscript. University of Nebraska.
121. Lemon, N. (1975). Linguistic developments and conceptualisati-
on. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 6, 173-188.
122. Lemon, N. and Warren, N. (1974). Salience, centrality and
self-relevance of traits in construing others. British Journal of social and
clinical Psychology 13, 119-124.
123. Lester, D. (1971). Cognitive complexity of the suicidal patient.
Psychological Reports 28, 158.
124. Levy, L. H. (1956). Personal consructs and predictive behavio-
ur. Journal of abnormal and social psychology 53, 54-58.
125. Levy, L. H. and Dugan, R. D. (1956). A factorial study in
personal constructs. Journal of Consulting Psychology 20, 53-57.
126. Levy, P. (1972). Concept-scale interaction in semantic differenti-
al research: solutions in search of a problem. British Journal of
Psychology 63, 235-236.
127. Lidz, T. (1964). "The Family and Human Adaptation". Hogarth.
128. Lifshitz, M. (1974). Quality professionals: does training make a
difference? A personal construct theory study of the issue. British Journal
of social and clinical Psychology 13, 183-189.
129. Lifshitz, M. (1976). Long range effects of fathers loss: the
cognitive complexity of bereaved children and their school adjustment.
British Journal of medical Psychology 49, 189-197.
130. Little, В. R. (1967). Age and sex differences in the use of
psychological, role and physicalistic constructs. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Oxford.
131. Little, B. R. (1968), Factors affecting the use of psychological
versus non-psychological constructs on the Rep. Test. Bulletin of the
British Psychological Society 21, 34.
132. McFadyen, M. and Foulds, G. A. (1972). Comparison of
provided and elicited grid content in the grid test of schizophrenic thought
disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 121, 53-57.
133. MePherson, F. M. (1969). Thought-process disorder, delusions
of persecution ad "non-integration" in schizophrenia. British Journal on
medical Psychology 42, 55-57.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29